Chapter 4 References
References
Macchi, C. V., Turati, C., & Simion, F. (2004). Can a Nonspecific Bias Toward Top-Heavy Patterns Explain Newborns’ Face Preference? Psychological Science, 15(6), 379–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00688.x
Mennella, J. A., Jagnow, C. P., & Beauchamp, G. K. (2001). Prenatal and postnatal flavor learning by human infants. Pediatrics, 107(6), E88. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.107.6.e88
Muenssinger, J., Matuz, T., Schleger, F., Kiefer-Schmidt, I., Goelz, R., Wacker-Gussmann, A., Birbaumer, N., & Preissl, H. (2013). Auditory habituation in the fetus and neonate: An fMEG study. Developmental Science, 16(2), 287–295. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12025
Quinn, P. C., Uttley, L., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith, M., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2008). Infant preference for female faces occurs for same- but not other-race faces. Journal of Neuropsychology, 2(1), 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1348/174866407×231029
Rennels, J. L., Juvrud, J., Kayl, A. J., Asperholm, M., Gredebäck, G., & Herlitz, A. (2017). Caregiving experience and its relation to perceptual narrowing of face gender. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1437–1446. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000335
Schaal, B., Marlier, L., & Soussignan, R. (2000). Human foetuses learn odours from their pregnant mother’s diet. Chemical Senses, 25(6), 729–737. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/25.6.729
Spangler, S. M., Schwarzer, G., Freitag, C., Vierhaus, M., Teubert, M., Fassbender, I., Lohaus, A., Kolling, T., Graf, F., Goertz, C., Knopf, M., Lamm, B., & Keller, H. (2013). The other-race effect in a longitudinal sample of 3-, 6- and 9-month-old infants: Evidence of a training effect. Infancy, 18: 516–533. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00137.x